Behavioural reactivity to potential threat is used to experimentally refine models of anxiety symptoms in rodents. We present a short review of the literature tying the most commonly used tasks to model anxiety symptoms to functional recruitment of bed nucleus of the stria terminalis circuits (BNST). Using a review of studies that investigated the role of the BNST in anxiety-like behaviour in rodents, we flag the certain challenges for the field. These stem from inconsistent methods of reporting the neuroanatomical BNST subregions and the interpretations of specific behaviour across a wide variety of tasks as ‘anxiety-like’. Finally, to assist in interpretation of the findings, we discuss the potential interactions between typically used ‘anxiety’ tasks of innate behaviour that are potentially modulated by the social and individual experience of the animal.

Some concepts of emotional experience used in the neuroscientific literature are both intuitive but nonetheless difficult to experimentally define, including stress, fear and notably anxiety. What are diagnosed as anxiety disorders have fluctuated over time, remarkably in light of what neurobiological experimental evidence has demonstrated to be associated with particular symptoms that characterise specific spectra of disorders of anxiety, such as trauma, stressor-related or obsessive-compulsive disorders [1]. Attempts to map particular brain systems to a rodent behavioural readout that could model symptoms of anxiety have been met with many challenges in terms of establishing a predictive theoretical framework [2,3]. A practical distinction for rodent experimental design between fear and anxiety driven defensive behaviour (discussed below) can be made from the nature of the physical external trigger. Fear is argued to be triggered by perception of an obvious or imminent threat, whereas anxiety is thought to represent the anticipation of an ambiguous or distant threat [4–6]. The duration of exposure to the threat has also been used to define brief presentations as fear-eliciting, whereas sustained presentations would elicit anxiety [7]. Clinical studies have also used the threat imminence, or duration, as protocol parameters to refine what brain regions become recruited by anxiety rather than fear [7,8]. One such area that did not prominently feature in the earlier neuropsychological models of anxiety is a region known as the Bed Nucleus — or Nuclei, given the complex collection of substructures — of the Stria Terminalis (BNST).

Studies indicated that the Bed Nuclei of the Stria Terminalis (BNST) may be preferentially recruited over the amygdala nuclei for anxiety-evoking rather than fear-evoking stimuli [9–11]. Both associative aversive learning protocols [8], and stimuli that evoke ‘innate’ anxiety such as phobic images of spiders have been used to investigate recruitment of BNST activity in humans [12]. Despite the attraction of a brain region being more strongly associated with responses defined as anxiety-like, there remains debate about how real a biological difference there is in the circuitry underlying what we define operationally as fear or anxiety [13]. Herein, we focus the review on studies using mice or rats that explore targeting the BNST with a behavioural readout that is argued to model symptoms of anxiety.

In laboratory rodents, the BNST is posited to have a role in innate responses to a stressor, such as brightly lit environment or predator odour [14–16] and in cases where the stressor potentiates subsequent startle responses [17,18]. Nonetheless, evidence for a role for BNST in the encoding of a conditioned experience is growing [19,20]. The nature of cue — discrete or contextual — used to trigger either conditioned freezing or flight behaviour was initially thought to control the involvement of the BNST, however the refinement of the task protocols by modulation of cue predictability, as well as its physical properties, has provided a more nuanced interpretation of when the BNST contributes to anxiety-related behaviour [21–24].

A significant challenge remains the limited resolution of the subnuclei of the BNST in human functional magnetic resonance imaging [9]. The BNST is a limbic forebrain structure, found encapsulating the anterior commissure, which is considered to be part of the so-called ‘extended amygdala’ and is well connected to form part of a threat detection system [25,26]. We will not review the details of the chemoarchitecture of the BNST subnuclei herein, for which we direct the reader to thorough reviews on the topic [7,25,26]. Despite progress on refining the connectivity of the BNST subnuclei, a lack of convention in terminology for naming subregions limits comparison across interventional studies [26,27] (see Figure 1). Moreover, there is evidence in the literature that the BNST may be sexually dimorphic (with functional consequences) both in rats [28,29], mice [15,30,31] and in humans [32,33]. It should be noted also that resources like the neuroanatomical atlases commonly used for reference and reporting, such as those published by Paxinos, Watson & Calabrese (first edition 1982), were generated using male animals.

Figure 1.

A representation of the anatomical focus of the rodent literature with regards to reporting a role of the BNST in anxiety-like behaviour.

The vast majority (31.7%) of papers do not specify a particular region (‘Unspecified’), referring to the BNST as a whole structure. Note that this representation may refer to the same anatomical area with different terminology used in the study. Subregions are grouped by the categorisation of Dong and Swanson [27] to aid standardisation.

Figure 1.

A representation of the anatomical focus of the rodent literature with regards to reporting a role of the BNST in anxiety-like behaviour.

The vast majority (31.7%) of papers do not specify a particular region (‘Unspecified’), referring to the BNST as a whole structure. Note that this representation may refer to the same anatomical area with different terminology used in the study. Subregions are grouped by the categorisation of Dong and Swanson [27] to aid standardisation.

Close modal

To illustrate the array of terms that were used to describe the anatomical subdivisions of the BNST and the variety of tasks used to investigate anxiety mechanisms, we performed a systematic literature review of recent rodent studies. Searches were conducted using PubMed, the Cochrane Library and pre-print archives (BioRxiv, MedRxiv, PscyArXiv) using the Boolean operators:

(((BNST) OR (Bed nucleus stria terminalis)) AND ((anxiety) OR (‘sustained fear’)) AND ((‘rodent’) OR (‘mice’) OR (‘rat’))”

This generated 584 unique literature records (published from 1997–2021). Pre-prints were included as the purpose was to sample what terms and tasks are being actively used in recent studies of the BNST. Two authors screened the papers with exclusion criteria that articles should be open access, in English, and have investigated a functional role of the BNST in tasks proposed to model anxiety-like behaviour (see Supplementary Figure S1). A total of 173 records were deemed appropriate for inclusion based on their relevance to anxiety-like behaviour and the BNST (to cut off date of 28th of February 2021). Most (but surprisingly not all) papers reported the sex of rodent subjects, although few included both sexes (27.8%) the majority were male-only studies (2.6 : 1 records). In the many studies that performed interventions or measured outcomes from the BNST area, the majority did not specify a particular subregion and analysed the structure taken as a whole (see Figure 1). On examination of the variety of terms used to describe subregions, what becomes apparent is the need for precise reporting of anatomical sites of measures in a consistent manner in order to compare across sexes, species and studies.

The validation of a task for translational potential is complex, including for pharmacological predictive validity for complex neuropsychiatric disorders like anxiety [34–36]. Another evident impression gained from the systematic review of the recent literature was that many distinct tasks were used to investigate BNST function in anxiety-like behaviour (Figure 2). The majority of the rodent studies featured as primary tasks were those that are based on innate or untrained experience of aversive (bright or open) space, i.e. the elevated plus maze (17.8% of reports) or open field maze task (12.9%). The next largest categories were tasks that use aversive associative experience i.e. Pavlovian threat/fear conditioning tasks. Below we expand on the role individual experience may play in the sensitivity and utility of these favoured tasks to model what they refer to as anxiety-like behaviour.

Analysis of recent literature reports of behavioural task used to elucidate BNST function in anxiety-related behaviour.

Figure 2.
Analysis of recent literature reports of behavioural task used to elucidate BNST function in anxiety-related behaviour.

Note that some papers utilised multiple tasks, each of which has been given a count of one. ‘Other’ interventions include those task variants used fewer than four times across the entirety of the literature. Above the bars are the absolute number of uses of each intervention across the referenced literature.

Figure 2.
Analysis of recent literature reports of behavioural task used to elucidate BNST function in anxiety-related behaviour.

Note that some papers utilised multiple tasks, each of which has been given a count of one. ‘Other’ interventions include those task variants used fewer than four times across the entirety of the literature. Above the bars are the absolute number of uses of each intervention across the referenced literature.

Close modal

For over a century, scientists have used mazes to study rodents’ behaviours. Mazes provide key insights into both innate and learned behaviours, as they show how a rodent instinctively navigates through a space, and additionally aids in an understanding of the mechanisms behind learning and memory. One of the first rodent mazes detailed by Willard Small in 1901 was rooted in a desire to understand Darwinian-inspired innate behaviours, and ‘to make observations upon the free expression of the animal's mental processes' [37].

The Elevated Plus Maze (EPM), pioneered by Pellow and colleagues [38], has been claimed to measure anxiety-like agoraphobia [39]. The EPM consists of two enclosed arms and two open arms arranged in a plus shape, and is commonly used to assess anxiolytic drugs to measure their efficacy (for review, see [40]). Similar to the EPM is the elevated T-maze, in which a rodent can explore two open arms and one enclosed arm [41]. These mazes are similarly useful as researchers surmise that anxiety-like behaviours can be induced by height, exposure to a new environment, and open spaces [42]. Generally, rodents tend to avoid open arms that could be dangerous for them, and exploration of these open arms is argued to be associated with lower levels of anxiety.

Another common and ethologically relevant test is the Open Field Maze (OFM), pioneered by Hall [43]. It is particularly useful as it is quite straightforward in its design; it consists of a wall-enclosed area that an animal cannot escape from. Laboratory rodents are innately averse to unknown, open, and illuminated environments, so when they are placed in the maze, their amount of locomotion and exploration is measured. Additional behaviours such as thigmotaxis (wall-hugging behaviour) and rearing can be measured to further indicate levels of anxiety-like behaviour [44].

While mazes offer some advantages over operant behavioural testing as the rodents perform more natural behaviours, there exist numerous limitations and challenges [35,45]. A potential factor for consideration is that rodents have a natural proclivity to explore [46]. As the environment is novel it should trigger an innate motivation to explore for resources or potential escape. However, with repeated testing of an experienced animal the memory of the context could influence responding read as anxiety-like behaviour [47]. The novelty-suppressed feeding test (NSF) measures another form of competing motivation, the hunger for a feeding opportunity against hyponeophagia, the neophobia induced by a new environment, which supresses an animal's feeding behaviour. The latency for food intake in a novel environment after being food-deprived is measured (for review, see [48]). Notably, the NSF test is affected by anxiolytic drugs such as barbiturates and benzodiazepines as these have been shown to decrease hyponeophagia [46].

Although mazes provide one aspect of studying ethological measures, in order to fully understand the development of anxiety-like behaviour it is useful to study acquired aversions [49]. In so-called fear/threat conditioning, an animal learns to directly associate a neutral stimulus with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US) and the conditioned response is measured [50]. In rodents, the most common fear conditioning protocols involve an electric foot shock paired with different sensory modalities including contextual, auditory, or visual cues [36,51–54]. Notably, olfaction is the key sensory modality used by rodents for identification of other animals, foraging, reproduction and social interaction [54,55] but relatively fewer studies use this modality. Olfactory fear conditioning uses an initially neutral odour (the Conditioned Stimulus, CS) paired with a foot shock (the US), and multiple studies have revealed the remarkable strength of olfactory conditioning over other senses even with the same intensity of aversive stimulus [56]. During foot-shock conditioning, the sensory information regarding the modality specific stimulus and unpleasant sensation are thought to be supported by the amygdala nuclei [57,58], which notably have reciprocal connections with the BNST [27,56]. This is in contrast with how innate predator odours have a dependence on the BNST but not the amygdala [59].

These type of classical conditioning protocols are used to typically measure conditioned freezing [60]. This has sometimes led to perhaps an over simplification of freezing behaviour to be equated with a fear-like response and the avoidance behaviour seen in mazes to be reported as simplified to an anxiety-like response. Others have discussed the defence behaviour repertoire of rodents in terms of active and passive responding [61,62]. However, given the expanding literature demonstrating the flexibility of rodent responses that are appropriate to the physical and psychological (predictability) nature of the threat cue and environment, it is unlikely that one form of behaviour is faithful to one emotional state or mood.

In rodents, a variety of aversive tasks, mazes or associative training, can evoke overlapping behavioural responses including avoidance, elevated heart rate, freezing, hypoactivity, suppressed food consumption, and increased vigilance [90]. In addition, laboratory rodents emit ultrasonic vocalisations (USVs, [63,64]). In rats, calls characterised at frequencies of ∼22 kHz are associated with aversive experience [65]. Measurements of USVs have been used as a correlate of stress and anxiety, which corroborates with the fact they can be reduced or blocked entirely by anxiogenic drugs [66,67]. Additionally, inter-individual variability in 22 kHz calls has commonly been reported. However, further analysis and insight into this variability has been lacking as some studies report an initial screening test to select only the animals that call (for review, see [68]). A study conducted by Brunelli ([69]) selectively bred rats on the basis extreme rates of USVs, and found rats in the genetic line with higher USVs were consistent with an altered affective phenotype such as vocalisations to touch in a new environment and low performance in a forced swim test; additionally, higher USV rates in infancy related to increased heart rate reactivity in adulthood [69]. Another study that selectively bred rats based on USV rates in infancy found that third generation rats from the low USV line spent more time in the open arms of the EPM [70]. Borta et al. ([71]) reported that vocalisation after foot-shock conditioning was more likely in rats who had spent more time in the closed arms of the EPM. However, they found that rats did not vocalise when they were in the EPM, which supports that observation that 22 kHz vocalisation is closely tied to conditioned freezing behaviour [71].

USVs may not be simply reflexive alarm calls, as rats seem to use them for communication as well, with 22 kHz calls typically being admitted in aversive situations and 55 kHz calls being admitted during appetitive situations [68]. Demaestri et al. ([72]) found that USV playback of aversive 22 kHz calls activated distinct patterns of cFos in the BNST and increased the rats’ avoidance behaviour in an elevated maze [72]. Thus, hearing USVs is also capable of inducing defensive behavioural changes in rats.

Rodents are social animals, and direct exposure to a threat is not the only way for an animal to learn and escape from danger. Thus, fear learning often occurs within a social context as an individual observes fear in other individuals. Social fear learning (SFL), otherwise known as vicarious fear learning, is a phenomenon in which an individual learns transmission of threat information through observation of a conspecific [73]. In rodents, pups can learn threat responses from their mothers as soon as they are born through SFL [74]. Later in life, mice can be fear conditioned through observation of a conspecific; Jeon et al. ([75]) showed that mice acquired fear vicariously through observing other mice reacting to aversive stimuli. An observer mouse that watched a demonstrator mouse receive foot shocks then displayed freezing behaviour as it watched the demonstrator react to foot shocks. When the demonstrator mouse was a sibling of the observer mouse, the observers exhibited increased freezing relative to observers unrelated to the conspecific. Thus, there are multiple factors at play regarding social fear learning in rodents, as the results of the relatedness between conspecifics imply that there is more than only emotional contagion involved in observational learning [75].

When housed together, rodents naturally form inter-individual hierarchies, which are important to consider when studying behavioural responses to potential threats. As early as 1938, scientists have observed and documented the presence of different social hierarchies in male mice [76]. Social dominance is defined as the animals in a social group that are most able to achieve a desirable goal, such as food or sex [77]. Evidence for variability in behaviour due to dominance hierarchies has widely increased [78], and thus, for many behavioural experiments, it is first helpful to gain an understanding of the social hierarchies between animals that are co-housed, including dominance hierarchies. In order to observe social dominance, scientists place rodents in situations that typically have predictable behavioural outcomes; for example, the submissive rodent may retreat while the dominant rodent will chase [79]. Multiple assays are employed to measure social dominance, such as a tube-test, resource competition task, or urine marking assay [80].

Recent studies have underscored differences between the anxiety-like behaviour of dominant and submissive cage-mates in both mice and rats. A meta-analysis and systematic review of the relationship between social dominance status and common behavioural phenotypes in male laboratory mice found little evidence for systematic phenotypic differences between dominant and submissive male mice [81]. Nonetheless, differential gene expression between dominant and submissive mice has been reported, yet social dominance was stable across time and not related to basal differences in mood, stress, or other physical features [82]. In contrast, submissive but not dominant mice were predisposed to anhedonia after chronic stress from restraint, tail suspension, and rat exposure [83]. Another study showed that the male offspring of dominant or submissive female mice performed differently in the EPM if they their respective dam had been exposed to prenatal restraint stress. Stressed submissive dam offspring showed the least exploration of the open arms [84]. However, it was also demonstrated that mice exposed to a chronic stress regime exhibited hyperlocomotion that was increased by triggers such as light intensity during experiments, which could impact measures of arm exploration in elevated mazes [85]. Indeed in the open field, submissive mice were reported to travel a further distance than dominant mice [78], which was regardless of sex; this could be due to hyperlocomotion from such aforementioned triggers. The same study found that social dominance accounted for phenotypic variation, but cage-identity barely accounted for any variation. Although these findings are consistent with other findings implicating social dominance's role in forming phenotypic traits in individual mice, they contradict with the findings implying the importance of cage-identity, pointing to common intra-cage variation [78]. This adds to an already mixed literature regarding social dominance and exploration. In rats, the importance of intra-cage dominant relationships was directly examined for USVs [86] and for fear conditioning by proxy; in which an animal learns from a conspecific to fear a threat cue by direct contact [87]. Submissive rats learned from dominant rats that a cue was threatening, as they displayed increased freezing to the cue. The authors found that the behaviours between cage mates as well as social dominance hierarchies in rats were predictive of social fear transmission.

It should be noted that we have discussed literature from both mice and rats herein, and there are noted differences in their behaviour and biology that is related to anxiety [88]. Moreover, the majority of studies continue to focus on readouts from male animals. The social housing and recent experience of rodents can be important influencing factors to consider. Therefore, careful attention must be taken before generalisation of findings across species, strain and sex.

Mazes that measure exploration of novel aversive contexts continue to be widely utilised as a valid approach to assess behaviour akin to anxiety despite certain limitations [35]. It is worth noting that even tasks that might superficially appear to measure similar behaviours, like the EPM and the OFM, do not necessarily always reveal the same trends of effects from interventions [35,89]. Encouragingly, more studies now characterise behaviour across multiple tasks, which may provide a clearer picture of a more ethological behaviour profile.

From a recent analysis of the literature, it becomes clear that the number of reports of interventions targeting the BNST to modulate anxiety-related behaviour is growing. Given the variability in reporting of sites of manipulation or regions where activity is correlated to behaviour, there is a need for consensus on how to refine the reporting of neuroanatomical locations. Despite these technical difficulties in comparison across studies, the BNST has certainly come more to the forefront as a target region for investigation in anxiety research [25].

  • Neuroanatomical evidence ties the BNST to anxiety-driven responses, but the role of distinct subregions remains unclear.

  • Conceptions of how to experimentally define anxiety-like behaviour have evolved over time and many different tasks are used in rodents.

  • Performance in typical tasks to measure ‘anxiety’ like behaviour may be sensitive to individual and social experience of the animal.

The authors declare that there are no competing interests associated with the manuscript.

E.N.C receives funding a collaborative research grant with Boehringer Ingelheim Pharma GmbH & Co.

Open access for this article was enabled by the participation of University of Bristol in an all-inclusive Read & Publish agreement with Portland Press and the Biochemical Society under a transformative agreement with JISC.

BNST

bed nuclei of the stria terminalis

EPM

elevated plus maze

NSF

novelty-suppressed feeding test

OFM

open field maze

SFL

social fear learning

US

unconditioned stimulus

USVs

ultrasonic vocalisations

1
Crocq
,
M.A.
(
2015
)
A history of anxiety: from hippocrates to DSM
.
Dialogues Clin. Neurosci.
17
,
319
325
2
Gray
,
J.A.
(
1982
)
Precis of The neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system
.
Behav. Brain Sci.
5
,
492
493
3
Stanford
,
S.C.
(
2017
)
Confusing preclinical (predictive) drug screens with animal “models” of psychiatric disorders, or “disorder-like” behaviour, is undermining confidence in behavioural neuroscience
.
J. Psychopharmacol.
31
,
641
643
4
Fanselow
,
M.
,
Angeles
,
L.
and
Lester
,
L.S.
(
1988
)
A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior, in
Evol. Learn.
(1st ed.), pp.
197
224
5
Fanselow
,
M.S.
(
1986
)
Associative vs topographical accounts of the immediate shock-freezing deficit in rats: implications for the response selection rules governing species-specific defensive reactions
.
Learn. Motiv.
17
,
16
39
6
McNaughton
,
N.
and
Corr
,
P.J.
(
2004
)
A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: fear/anxiety and defensive distance
.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
28
,
285
305
7
Davis
,
M.
,
Walker
,
D.L.
,
Miles
,
L.
and
Grillon
,
C.
(
2010
)
Phasic vs sustained fear in rats and humans: role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
35
,
105
135
8
Robinson
,
O.J.
,
Vytal
,
K.
,
Cornwell
,
B.R.
and
Grillon
,
C.
(
2013
)
The impact of anxiety upon cognition: perspectives from human threat of shock studies
.
Front. Hum. Neurosci.
7
,
1
21
9
Avery
,
S.N.
,
Clauss
,
J.A.
and
Blackford
,
J.U.
(
2016
)
The human BNST: functional role in anxiety and addiction
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
41
,
126
141
10
Klumpers
,
F.
,
Kroes
,
M.C.W.
,
Baas
,
J.M.P.
and
Fernández
,
G.
(
2017
)
How human amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis may drive distinct defensive responses
.
J. Neurosci.
37
,
9645
9656
11
Luyck
,
K.
,
Goode
,
T.D.
,
Lee Masson
,
H.
and
Luyten
,
L.
(
2019
)
Distinct activity patterns of the human Bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and amygdala during fear learning
.
Neuropsychol. Rev.
29
,
181
185
12
Siminski
,
N.
,
Borgmann
,
L.
,
Becker
,
M.P.I.
,
Hofmann
,
D.
,
Gathmann
,
B.
,
Leehr
,
E.J.
et al (
2021
)
Centromedial amygdala is more relevant for phobic confrontation relative to the bed nucleus of stria terminalis in patients with spider phobia
.
J. Psychiatr. Res.
143
,
268
275
13
Daniel-Watanabe
,
L.
and
Fletcher
,
P.C.
(
2021
)
Are fear and anxiety truly distinct?
Biol. Psychiatry Glob. Open Sci.
2
,
341
349
14
Yamauchi
,
N.
,
Takahashi
,
D.
,
Sugimura
,
Y.K.
,
Kato
,
F.
,
Amano
,
T.
and
Minami
,
M.
(
2018
)
Activation of the neural pathway from the dorsolateral bed nucleus of the stria terminalis to the central amygdala induces anxiety-like behaviors
.
Eur. J. Neurosci.
48
,
3052
3061
15
Giardino
,
W.J.
,
Eban-Rothschild
,
A.
,
Christoffel
,
D.J.
,
Bin
,
L.S.
,
Malenka
,
R.C.
and
de Lecea
,
L.
(
2018
)
Parallel circuits from the bed nuclei of stria terminalis to the lateral hypothalamus drive opposing emotional states
.
Nat. Neurosci.
21
,
1084
1095
16
Bruzsik
,
B.
,
Biro
,
L.
,
Sarosdi
,
K.R.
,
Zelena
,
D.
,
Sipos
,
E.
,
Szebik
,
H.
et al (
2021
)
Neurochemically distinct populations of the bed nucleus of stria terminalis modulate innate fear response to weak threat evoked by predator odor stimuli
.
Neurobiol. Stress
15
,
100415
17
Kim
,
S.Y.
,
Adhikari
,
A.
,
Lee
,
S.Y.
,
Marshel
,
J.H.
,
Kim
,
C.K.
,
Mallory
,
C.S.
et al (
2013
)
Diverging neural pathways assemble a behavioural state from separable features in anxiety
.
Nature
496
,
219
223
18
Walker
,
D.L.
,
Toufexis
,
D.J.
and
Davis
,
M.
(
2003
)
Role of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis versus the amygdala in fear, stress, and anxiety
.
Eur. J. Pharmacol.
463
,
199
216
19
Bjorni
,
M.
,
Rovero
,
N.G.
,
Yang
,
E.R.
,
Holmes
,
A.
and
Halladay
,
L.R.
(
2020
)
Phasic signaling in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis during fear learning predicts within- And across-session cued fear expression
.
Learn. Mem.
27
,
83
90
20
Bruzsik
,
B.
,
Biro
,
L.
,
Zelena
,
D.
,
Sipos
,
E.
,
Szebik
,
H.
,
Sarosdi
,
K.R.
et al (
2021
)
Somatostatin neurons of the bed nucleus of stria terminalis enhance associative fear memory consolidation in mice
.
J. Neurosci.
41
,
1982
1995
21
Goode
,
T.D.
,
Ressler
,
R.L.
,
Acca
,
G.M.
,
Miles
,
O.W.
and
Maren
,
S.
(
2019
)
Bed nucleus of the stria terminalis regulates fear to unpredictable threat signals
.
eLife
8
,
e46525
22
Goode
,
T.D.
and
Maren
,
S.
(
2017
)
Role of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in aversive learning and memory
.
Learn. Mem.
24
,
480
491
23
Goode
,
T.D.
,
Acca
,
G.M.
and
Maren
,
S.
(
2020
)
Threat imminence dictates the role of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in contextual fear
.
Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.
167
,
107116
24
Totty
,
M.S.
,
Warren
,
N.
,
Huddleston
,
I.
,
Ramanathan
,
K.R.
,
Ressler
,
R.L.
,
Oleksiak
,
C.R.
et al (
2021
)
Behavioral and brain mechanisms mediating conditioned flight behavior in rats
.
Sci. Rep
11
,
8215
25
Lebow
,
M.A.
and
Chen
,
A.
(
2016
)
Overshadowed by the amygdala: the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis emerges as key to psychiatric disorders
.
Mol. Psychiatry
21
,
450
463
26
Dong
,
H.W.
,
Petrovich
,
G.D.
,
Watts
,
A.G.
and
Swanson
,
L.W.
(
2001
)
Basic organization of projections from the oval and fusiform nuclei of the bed nuclei of the stria terminalis in adult rat brain
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
436
,
430
455
27
Dong
,
H.W.
,
Petrovich
,
G.D.
and
Swanson
,
L.W.
(
2001
)
Topography of projections from amygdala to bed nuclei of the stria terminalis
.
Brain Res. Rev.
38
,
192
246
28
Toufexis
,
D.
(
2007
)
Region- and sex-specific modulation of anxiety behaviours in the rat
.
J. Neuroendocrinol.
19
,
461
473
29
Urien
,
L.
and
Bauer
,
E.P.
(
2022
)
Sex differences in BNST and amygdala activation by contextual, cued, and unpredictable threats
.
eNeuro
9
,
ENEURO.0233-21.2021
30
Yu
,
W.
,
Caira
,
C.M.
,
Sanchez
,
N.D.R.
,
Moseley
,
G.A.
and
Kash
,
T.L.
(
2021
)
Corticotropin-releasing factor neurons in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis exhibit sex-specific pain encoding in mice
.
. Sci. Rep.
11
,
12500
31
Bayless
,
D.W.
,
Yang
,
T.
,
Mason
,
M.M.
,
Susanto
,
A.A.T.
,
Lobdell
,
A.
and
Shah
,
N.M.
(
2019
)
Limbic neurons shape sex recognition and social behavior in sexually naive males
.
Cell
176
,
1190
1205.e20
32
Allen
,
L.S.
and
Gorski
,
R.A.
(
1990
)
Sex difference in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis of the human brain
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
302
,
697
706
33
Avery
,
S.N.
,
Clauss
,
J.A.
,
Winder
,
D.G.
,
Woodward
,
N.
,
Heckers
,
S.
and
Blackford
,
J.U.
(
2014
)
BNST neurocircuitry in humans
.
Neuroimage
91
,
311
323
34
Stanford
,
S.C.
(
2020
)
Some reasons why preclinical studies of psychiatric disorders fail to translate: what can be rescued from the misunderstanding and misuse of animal ‘models’?
Altern. Lab. Anim.
48
,
106
115
35
Ennaceur
,
A.
(
2014
)
Tests of unconditioned anxiety: pitfalls and disappointments
.
Physiol. Behav.
135
,
55
71
36
Bali
,
A.
and
Jaggi
,
A.S.
(
2015
)
Electric foot shock stress: a useful tool in neuropsychiatric studies
.
Rev. Neurosci.
26
,
655
677
37
Small
,
W.S.
(
1901
)
Experimental study of the mental processes of the rat
.
Am. J. Psychol.
12
,
206
239
38
Pellow
,
S.
,
Chopin
,
P.
,
File
,
S.E.
and
Briley
,
M.
(
1985
)
Validation of open: closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat
.
J. Neurosci Methods
14
,
149
167
39
Walf
,
A.A.
and
Frye
,
C.A.
(
2007
)
The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behavior in rodents
.
Nat. Protoc.
2
,
322
328
40
Himanshu
,
Dharmila
,
S.D.
and
Nutan,
. (
2020
)
A review of behavioral tests to evaluate different types of anxiety and anti-anxiety effects
.
Clin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci.
18
,
341
351
41
Graeff
,
F.G.
,
Ferreira Netto
,
C.
and
Zangrossi Jr
,
H.
(
1998
)
The elevated T-maze as an experimental model of anxiety
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
23
,
237
246
42
Shoji
,
H.
and
Miyakawa
,
T.
(
2021
)
Effects of test experience, closed-arm wall color, and illumination level on behavior and plasma corticosterone response in an elevated plus maze in Male C57BL/6J mice: a challenge against conventional interpretation of the test
.
Mol. Brain
14
,
34
43
Hall
,
C.S.
(
1934
)
Emotional behavior in the rat. I. defecation and urination as measures of individual differences in emotionality
.
J. Comp. Psychol.
18
,
385
403
44
Seibenhener
,
M.L.
and
Wooten
,
M.C.
(
2015
)
Use of the open field maze to measure locomotor and anxiety-like behavior in mice
.
J. Vis. Exp.
96
,
e52434
45
d'Isa
,
R.
,
Comi
,
G.
and
Leocani
,
L.
(
2021
)
Apparatus design and behavioural testing protocol for the evaluation of spatial working memory in mice through the spontaneous alternation T-maze
.
Sci. Rep.
11
,
21177
46
Lueptow
,
L.M.
(
2017
)
Novel object recognition test for the investigation of learning and memory in mice
.
J. Vis. Exp.
126
,
55718
47
Carobrez
,
A.P.
and
Bertoglio
,
L.J.
(
2005
)
Ethological and temporal analyses of anxiety-like behavior: the elevated plus-maze model 20 years on
.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
29
,
1193
1205
48
Ramaker
,
M.J.
and
Dulawa
,
S.C.
(
2017
)
Identifying fast-onset antidepressants using rodent models
.
Mol. Psychiatry
22
,
656
665
49
Kim
,
J.J.
and
Jung
,
M.W.
(
2018
)
Fear paradigms: the times they are a-changin’
.
Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci.
24
,
38
43
50
LeDoux
,
J.E.
(
2014
)
Coming to terms with fear
.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
111
,
2871
2878
51
Dębiec
,
J.
and
Ledoux
,
J.E.
(
2004
)
Disruption of reconsolidation but not consolidation of auditory fear conditioning by noradrenergic blockade in the amygdala
.
Neuroscience
129
,
267
272
52
Shi
,
C.
and
Davis
,
M.
(
2001
)
Visual pathways involved in fear conditioning measured with fear-potentiated startle: behavioral and anatomic studies
.
J. Neurosci.
21
,
9844
9855
53
Wiltgen
,
B.J.
,
Sanders
,
M.J.
,
Behne
,
N.S.
and
Fanselow
,
M.S.
(
2001
)
Sex differences, context preexposure, and the immediate shock deficit in Pavlovian context conditioning with mice
.
Behav. Neurosci.
115
,
26
32
54
Kroon
,
J.A.V.
and
Carobrez
,
A.P.
(
2009
)
Olfactory fear conditioning paradigm in rats: effects of midazolam, propranolol or scopolamine
.
Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.
91
,
32
40
55
Restrepo
,
D.
,
Arellano
,
J.
,
Oliva
,
A.M.
,
Schaefer
,
M.L.
and
Lin
,
W.
(
2004
)
Emerging views on the distinct but related roles of the main and accessory olfactory systems in responsiveness to chemosensory signals in mice
.
Horm. Behav.
46
,
247
256
56
Hakim
,
M.
,
Battle
,
A.R.
,
Belmer
,
A.
,
Bartlett
,
S.E.
,
Johnson
,
L.R.
and
Chehrehasa
,
F.
(
2019
)
Pavlovian olfactory fear conditioning: its neural circuity and importance for understanding clinical fear-based disorders
.
Front. Mol. Neurosci.
19
,
12
57
Cousens
,
G.
and
Otto
,
T.
(
1998
)
Both pre- and posttraining excitotoxic lesions of the basolateral amygdala abolish the expression of olfactory and contextual fear conditioning
.
Behav. Neurosci.
112
,
1092
1103
58
Tovote
,
P.
,
Fadok
,
J.P.
and
Lüthi
,
A.
(
2015
)
Neuronal circuits for fear and anxiety
.
Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
16
,
317
331
59
Fendt
,
M.
,
Endres
,
T.
and
Apfelbach
,
R.
(
2003
)
Temporary inactivation of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis but not of the amygdala blocks freezing induced by trimethylthiazoline, a component of fox feces
.
J. Neurosci.
23
,
23
28
60
Blanchard
,
R.J.
and
Blanchard
,
D.C.
(
1969
)
Crouching as an index of fear
.
J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.
67
,
370
375
61
Gruene
,
T.M.
,
Flick
,
K.
,
Stefano
,
A.
,
Shea
,
S.D.
and
Shansky
,
R.M.
(
2015
)
Sexually divergent expression of active and passive conditioned fear responses in rats
.
eLife
4
,
e11352
62
Blanchard
,
R.J.
and
Blanchard
,
D.C.
(
1969
)
Passive and active reactions to fear-eliciting stimuli
.
J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.
1
,
129
135
63
Anderson
,
J.W.
(
1954
)
The production of ultrasonic sounds by laboratory rats and other mammals
.
Science
119
,
808
809
64
Sales
,
G.D.
(
2010
)
Ultrasonic calls of wild and wild-type rodents
.
Handb. Behav. Neurosci.
19
,
77
88
65
Wöhr
,
M.
,
Borta
,
A.
and
Schwarting
,
R.K.W.
(
2005
)
Overt behavior and ultrasonic vocalization in a fear conditioning paradigm: a dose-response study in the rat
.
Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.
3
,
228
240
66
Gardner
,
C.R.
(
1985
)
Distress vocalization in rat pups a simple screening method for anxiolytic drugs
.
J. Pharmacol. Methods
3
,
181
187
67
Sánchez
,
C.
(
2003
)
Stress-induced vocalisation in adult animals. A valid model of anxiety?
Eur. J. Pharmacol.
463
,
133
143
68
Schwarting
,
R.K.W.
and
Wöhr
,
M.
(
2012
)
On the relationships between ultrasonic calling and anxiety-related behavior in rats
.
Brazilian J. Med. Biol. Res.
45
,
337
348
69
Brunelli
,
S.A.
(
2005
)
Selective breeding for an infant phenotype: rat pup ultrasonic vocalization (USV)
.
Behav. Genet.
35
,
53
65
70
Dichter
,
G.S.
,
Brunelli
,
S.A.
and
Hofer
,
M.A.
(
1996
)
Elevated plus-maze behavior in adult offspring of selectively bred rats
.
Physiol. Behav.
1
,
299
304
71
Borta
,
A.
,
Wöhr
,
M.
and
Schwarting
,
R.
(
2006
)
Rat ultrasonic vocalization in aversively motivated situations and the role of individual differences in anxiety-related behavior
.
Behav. Brain Res.
2
,
271
280
72
Demaestri
,
C.
,
Brenhouse
,
H.C.
and
Honeycutt
,
J.A.
(
2019
)
22 kHz and 55 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations differentially influence neural and behavioral outcomes: implications for modeling anxiety via auditory stimuli in the rat
.
Behav. Brain Res.
360
,
134
145
73
Debiec
,
J.
and
Olsson
,
A.
(
2017
)
Social fear learning: from animal models to human function
.
Trends Cogn. Sci.
7
,
546
555
74
Debiec
,
J.
and
Sullivan
,
R.M.
(
2014
)
Intergenerational transmission of emotional trauma through amygdala-dependent mother-to-infant transfer of specific fear
.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
33
,
12222
7
75
Jeon
,
D.
,
Kim
,
S.
,
Chetana
,
M.
,
Jo
,
D.
,
Ruley
,
H.E.
,
Lin
,
S.-Y.
et al (
2010
)
Observational fear learning involves affective pain system and Cav1.2 Ca2+ channels in ACC
.
Nat. Neurosci.
4
,
482
488
76
Uhrich
,
J.
(
1938
)
The social hierarchy in albino mice
.
J. Comp. Psychol.
2
,
373
413
77
Baenninger
,
R.
and
Kraus
,
S.
(
1981
)
Some determinants of aggressive and predatory responses in Betta splendens
.
J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.
95
,
220
227
78
Varholick
,
J.A.
,
Pontiggia
,
A.
,
Murphy
,
E.
,
Daniele
,
V.
,
Palme
,
R.
,
Voelkl
,
B.
et al (
2019
)
Social dominance hierarchy type and rank contribute to phenotypic variation within cages of laboratory mice
.
Sci. Rep.
9
,
13650
79
Drews
,
C.
(
1993
)
The concept and definition of dominance in animal behaviour
.
Behaviour
125
,
283
313
80
Wang
,
F.
,
Kessels
,
H.W.
and
Hu
,
H.
(
2014
)
The mouse that roared: neural mechanisms of social hierarchy
.
Trends Neurosci.
37
,
674
682
81
Varholick
,
J.A.
,
Bailoo
,
J.D.
,
Jenkins
,
A.
,
Voelkl
,
B.
and
Würbel
,
H.
(
2021
)
A systematic review and meta-analysis of the relationship between social dominance status and common behavioral phenotypes in Male laboratory mice
.
Front. Behav. Neurosci.
14
,
624036
82
Pallé
,
A.
,
Zorzo
,
C.
,
Luskey
,
V.E.
,
Mcgreevy
,
K.R.
,
Fernández
,
S.
and
Trejo
,
J.L.
(
2019
)
Social dominance differentially alters gene expression in the medial prefrontal cortex without affecting adult hippocampal neurogenesis or stress and anxiety-like behavior
.
FASEB J.
33
,
6995
7008
83
Strekalova
,
T.
,
Spanagel
,
R.
,
Bartsch
,
D.
,
Henn
,
F.A.
and
Gass
,
P.
(
2004
)
Stress-induced anhedonia in mice is associated with deficits in forced swimming and exploration
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
29
,
2007
2017
84
Gross
,
M.
,
Romi
,
H.
,
Miller
,
A.
and
Pinhasov
,
A.
(
2018
)
Social dominance predicts hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor recruitment and resilience to prenatal adversity
.
Sci. Rep.
8
,
9595
85
Strekalova
,
T.
,
Spanagel
,
R.
,
Dolgov
,
O.
and
Bartsch
,
D.
(
2005
)
Stress-induced hyperlocomotion as a confounding factor in anxiety and depression models in mice
.
Behav. Pharmacol.
16
,
171
180
86
Inagaki
,
H.
,
Kuwahara
,
M.
,
Kikusui
,
T.
and
Tsubone
,
H.
(
2005
)
The influence of social environmental condition on the production of stress-induced 22 kHz calls in adult Male wistar rats
.
Physiol. Behav.
84
,
17
22
87
Jones
,
C.E.
and
Monfils
,
M.-H.
(
2016
)
Dominance status predicts social fear transmission in laboratory rats
.
Anim. Cogn.
19
,
1051
1069
88
Ellenbroek
,
B.
and
Youn
,
J.
(
2016
)
Rodent models in neuroscience research: is it a rat race?
Dis. Model. Mech.
9
,
1079
1087
89
Mazzone
,
C.M.
,
Pati
,
D.
,
Michaelides
,
M.
,
DiBerto
,
J.
,
Fox
,
J.H.
,
Tipton
,
G.
et al (
2018
)
Acute engagement of Gq-mediated signaling in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis induces anxiety-like behavior
.
Mol. Psychiatry
23
,
143
153
90
Lezak
,
K.R.
,
Missig
,
G.
,
Carlezon
,
W.A. Jr.
(
2017
)
Behavioral methods to study anxiety in rodents
.
Dialogues Clin Neurosci.
19
,
181
191
This is an open access article published by Portland Press Limited on behalf of the Biochemical Society and the Royal Society of Biology and distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License 4.0 (CC BY). Open access for this article was enabled by the participation of University of Bristol in an all-inclusive Read & Publish agreement with Portland Press and the Biochemical Society under a transformative agreement with JISC.

Supplementary data