Population-level lateralization in structure and function is a fundamental measure of the human nervous system. To what extent nonhuman primates exhibit similar patterns of asymmetry remains a topic of considerable scientific interest. In this mini-review, a brief summary of findings on brain asymmetries in nonhuman primates in brain regions considered to the homolog's to Broca's and Wernicke's area are presented. Limitations of existing and directions for future studies are discussed in the context of facilitating comparative investigations in primates.

A fundamental feature of the human brain is lateralization, which is defined as left-right differences neuroanatomy and neurofunctions that underlie different sensory, motor and cognitive functions [1–4]. Asymmetries in the structure are often conceptualized and quantified in terms of unilateral differences between homologous brain regions following a set of operationally defined neuroanatomical landmarks. More recent advances in neuroimaging techniques and quantification methods that operate at the level of the voxel or vertices have allowed for expansive, whole brain analyses of asymmetry in gray matter, white matter, cortical thickness, surface area and in anatomical and functional connectivity [5–9]. In humans, these approaches have revealed more comprehensive results with respect to whole brain variation in lateralization in morphology and connectivity.

In the study of behavioral and brain lateralization, scientists typically distinguish between individual and population-level asymmetries. Specifically, at the individual level, left-right difference measures obtained from bilateral traits are computed for each subject. The sign and absolute value indicate the direction and magnitude of a given subjects laterality and these are often described as asymmetry quotient (AQ) scores.

Population-level asymmetry indicates whether the average AQ scores for a sample of subjects differs from zero or, relatedly, whether a significant proportion of subjects within a sample show the same directional bias based on the sign of the AQ value. Thus, like other types of research on asymmetry, different distributions of lateralization can be obtained within a population or sample including: (1) a normal distribution of AQs, (2) a bimodal distribution AQ scores and (3) a rightward or leftward skewed distribution of AQ scores. Normal and bimodal distributions of AQ scores will have mean values on or about zero and reflect no population-level bias but for different reasons. In contrast, skewed distributions reflect population-level biases (depending on sample size and consistently of the asymmetry within the sample).

In the context of brain and behavioral asymmetries in nonhuman primates, it has been known for quite some time that individual subjects show asymmetries in certain behaviors, such as hand preferences or on measures of neuroanatomical structures [10–13]. What has been less documented and, indeed a matter of much greater debate, is whether nonhuman primates exhibit population-level asymmetries and, if they do, whether they are comparable in direction to those observed in humans. Indeed, the question of homology in population-level behavioral asymmetries has received considerable attention as it pertains to handedness [14–20]. However, there has been less research and thereby less discussion on the existence of population-level asymmetries in neuroanatomy in nonhuman primates and their similarities (and differences) to those observed in humans [21].

In this review, I summarize recently published data on asymmetries in the nonhuman primate brain that have utilized more modern imaging technologies, with a specific emphasis on studies in chimpanzees. Notably, many early studies on brain asymmetries focused on linear measurements of the length of cortical sulci measured directly from postmortem brains [22–26], endocasts [27–29] or in a few cases magnetic resonance images [30–32]. In addition, several studies quantified asymmetries in the shape of the skull based on endocasts or related methods [33–35]. These collective findings have been summarized recently by Hopkins et al. [21]. In this mini-review, I will summarize more recent findings on brain asymmetries in nonhuman primates that have utilized region-of-interest methods. Furthermore, I will limit the review to studies that have quantified brain regions considered homologous to Broca's and Wernicke's areas in the human brain (see Figure 1). Specially, some of the first studies to describe lateralization in structure and function came from case studies of patients with unilateral damage to the left hemisphere inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) or posterior superior temporal gyrus (STG). In these cases, the patients exhibited deficits in language production and/or language comprehension whereas patients with damage to these same brain regions in the right hemisphere did not show any speech deficits. This led to the view that language functions are largely controlled by the left hemisphere, a finding largely confirmed over the next century in additional clinical cases and from experimental studies using a variety of neuropsychological test and measure employing functional imaging. Because many believe that language is uniquely humans, the historical and contemporary interest in brain asymmetries has largely focused on quantifying measures of brain regions or sulci that are homologous to Broca's and Wernicke's area.

Morphological regions that comprise the planum temporale (left panel) and inferior frontal gyrus (right panel) of the human brain.

Figure 1.
Morphological regions that comprise the planum temporale (left panel) and inferior frontal gyrus (right panel) of the human brain.

OP = pars opercularis, Tr = pars triangularis.

Figure 1.
Morphological regions that comprise the planum temporale (left panel) and inferior frontal gyrus (right panel) of the human brain.

OP = pars opercularis, Tr = pars triangularis.

Close modal

Planum temporale

Brodmann's area 22 (BA22) or Wernicke's area is found in the posterior STG [36,37]. The most reliable landmark to quantify this region is referred to as the planum temporale (PT), which is the bank of tissue that lies posterior the Heschl's gyrus (see Figure 1). Historically, scientists have quantified either the surface area or the gray matter volume of the PT in humans and consistently reported a significant leftward asymmetry (PT) (see Figure 2a and Table 1). With respect to nonhuman primates, in one of the first systematic comparative studies of PT asymmetry in chimpanzees, Gannon et al. [38] measured the surface area from a sample of 18 postmortem brains and reported a leftward asymmetry in 17 of the apes. In three subsequent studies, the PT surface area was measured either directly from MRI scans or from 3D reconstructions of the sulci in samples of chimpanzees and all three studies reported a significant population-level leftward asymmetry [39–42]. More recently, Marie et al. [43] quantified the PT in a sample of 96 adult baboons and reported a population-level leftward asymmetry. Following on from this report, Becker et al. [44] measured the PT in a sample of 35 newborn baboons (<3 months of age) and similarly found a leftward asymmetry. Lastly, in rhesus monkeys, no population-level bias has been found, a somewhat interesting result in light of the results reported in baboons, another Old World primate species.

Figure 2.

Mean AQ scores (±s.e.) for (a) PT surface area and (b) PT gray matter volume in different primate species.

Figure 2.

Mean AQ scores (±s.e.) for (a) PT surface area and (b) PT gray matter volume in different primate species.

Close modal
Table 1.
Planum temporale asymmetries in nonhuman primates.
#L#A#RMean AQset
Surface area 
Chimpanzee 17 −0.574 0.132 −4.35** 
Chimpanzee 159 21 43 −0.129 0.013 −9.55** 
Chimpanzee 197 32 62 −0.104 0.013 −7.96** 
Chimpanzee NA   
Baboon 60 11 25 −0.070 0.013 −5.62** 
Baboon 25 −0.058 0.011 −5.15** 
Rhesus monkey 21 26 −0.013 0.035 −0.36 
Gray matter volume 
Human 34 12 NA   
Chimpanzee 134 12 43 0.107 0.013 8.00** 
Chimpanzee 198 25 68 −0.117 0.013 −8.98** 
Baboon 24 −0.121 0.028 −4.20** 
Rhesus monkey 10 0.040 0.042 0.95 
Bonnett monkey 11 −0.052 0.056  
Vervet monkey 16 17 −0.002 0.023  
Cytoarchitectonics 
Humans NA   
Chimpanzee (nn) 10 −0.401 0.141 −2.91* 
Chimpanzee (nd) −0.144 0.124 −1.17 
Chimpanzee (vol) −0.265 0.144 −1.89 
Chimpanzee (np) 0.000 0.049 0.97 
Rhesus (vol) NA   
#L#A#RMean AQset
Surface area 
Chimpanzee 17 −0.574 0.132 −4.35** 
Chimpanzee 159 21 43 −0.129 0.013 −9.55** 
Chimpanzee 197 32 62 −0.104 0.013 −7.96** 
Chimpanzee NA   
Baboon 60 11 25 −0.070 0.013 −5.62** 
Baboon 25 −0.058 0.011 −5.15** 
Rhesus monkey 21 26 −0.013 0.035 −0.36 
Gray matter volume 
Human 34 12 NA   
Chimpanzee 134 12 43 0.107 0.013 8.00** 
Chimpanzee 198 25 68 −0.117 0.013 −8.98** 
Baboon 24 −0.121 0.028 −4.20** 
Rhesus monkey 10 0.040 0.042 0.95 
Bonnett monkey 11 −0.052 0.056  
Vervet monkey 16 17 −0.002 0.023  
Cytoarchitectonics 
Humans NA   
Chimpanzee (nn) 10 −0.401 0.141 −2.91* 
Chimpanzee (nd) −0.144 0.124 −1.17 
Chimpanzee (vol) −0.265 0.144 −1.89 
Chimpanzee (np) 0.000 0.049 0.97 
Rhesus (vol) NA   

NA, not applicable or no data reported; se, standard error; n, neuron number; nd, neuron density; vol, volume.

In addition to the surface area, several authors have also measured the gray matter volume of the PT in nonhuman primates. Not surprisingly, the results for PT gray matter asymmetries are largely consistent with the surface area measures. This is because, assuming that cortical thickness in gray matter remains relatively consistent throughout the region, then the larger surface area will produce a larger gray matter volume. Thus, the leftward asymmetries observed for surface area in chimpanzees [41] and baboons [45] are similarly observed for gray matter volume (see Figure 2b and Table 1). In contrast, neither rhesus monkeys, bonnet monkeys or vervet monkeys show a population-level bias in gray matter volume for the PT [46] (see Figure 2b).

Cytoarchitectonics

There are remarkably few studies documenting asymmetries at the cellular level in BA22 in human and nonhuman primates (Table 1). In one of the only reported studies in humans, Galaburda et al. [47] reported a larger volume of BA22 in the left hemisphere of four postmortem brains. In chimpanzees, Spocter et al. [48] measured the volume, neuron number and neuron density of BA22 in 12 postmortem adult brains and reported a significant leftward asymmetry for each measure (see Table 1). In a follow up study, Spocter et al. [49] further quantified microstructural asymmetries in BA22 by measuring neuropil space and found no population-level bias. Finally, Gannon et al. [50] measured the volume of BA22 in 6 rhesus monkeys and found the 5 of the 6 individuals showed a leftward asymmetry.

Inferior frontal gyrus (Broca's area homolog)

Comparative studies of the IFG in nonhuman primates are challenging because of differences in the sulcal landmarks the define the region (see Figure 3). Specifically, in humans, the IFG is broadly classified into the Pars opercularis (Op) and the Pars triangularis (Tr). The Pars opercularis is bordered superiorly by the inferior frontal sulcus (ifs), anteriorly by the anterior ascending ramus of the lateral fissure (aalf) and posteriorly by the inferior precentral sulcus (iprs). The Tr is bordered inferiorly by the horizonal ramus of the lateral fissure (half) and posteriorly by the ascending ramus, and together these rami form a ‘triangle’ shaped gyrus [51–53]. In chimpanzees, the sulci landmarks that define the Op are homologous; however, chimpanzees lack a horizontal ramus of the lateral fissure which precludes being able to define the Tr using sulci landmarks. In more distantly related Old and New World monkeys, the cortical folds are even less similar further complicating the measurement. Cytoarchitectonic studies in macaques and baboons have shown that BA44 and BA45, the constituent cellular components that derive the Op and Tr, respectively, are buried in the central portion of the arcuate sulcus [54,55]. Thus, some have suggested that measurement of the entire or portions of arcuate sulcus can be used as approximations to the measurement of Broca's area, at least in Old World monkeys (discussed below).

Figure 3.

Sulci used to define the inferior frontal gyrus in humans (upper panel) and chimpanzees (lower panel). Ipcs = inferior precentral sulcus, ifs = inferior frontal sulcus, ar = ascending ramus, hr = horizontal ramus, ds = dimple sulcus, fos = fronto-orbital sulcus. From Keller et al. [58].

Figure 3.

Sulci used to define the inferior frontal gyrus in humans (upper panel) and chimpanzees (lower panel). Ipcs = inferior precentral sulcus, ifs = inferior frontal sulcus, ar = ascending ramus, hr = horizontal ramus, ds = dimple sulcus, fos = fronto-orbital sulcus. From Keller et al. [58].

Close modal

In three separate studies that have utilized region-of-interest methods, either no population-level or a slight rightward bias was reported in human subjects [56–58]. In a comparative study of the IFG gray matter volume in 30 human and 30 chimpanzees by Keller et al. [58], neither species showed a population-level bias (see Table 2). In one subsequent study measuring the total gray matter volume of the IFG in a much larger sample of chimpanzees (n = 189), Hopkins et al. [59] reported no population-level bias, consistent with the report by Keller et al. [58]. In contrast, studies by Cantalupo and Hopkins [60] reported a leftward asymmetry in the overall volume of the IFG in chimpanzees, bonobos and gorillas, albeit in much smaller samples of subjects.

Table 2.
Asymmetries in Broca area homolog regions: volumetrics and cytoarchitectonics.
#L#A#RMean AQset
Total volume 
Human 12 11 NA   
Human NA   −0.082 0.035 −2.34* 
Chimpanzee 30 23 −0.104 0.038 −2.71** 
Chimpanzee 87 12 90 0.021 0.030 0.68 
Bonobo −0.097 0.054 −1.77 
Gorilla −0.068 0.023 −2.91 
Gray matter volume 
Human 28 31 0.041 0.061 0.67 
Human 12 18 −0.021 0.112 −0.001 
Human 17 29 .084 0.074 1.13 
Chimpanzee 12 18 0.052 0.066 0.79 
Chimpanzee 87 12 90 0.021 0.030 0.68 
Cytoarchitectonic 
Human 
BA44 10 NA   
BA45 NA   
Chimpanzee 
BA44 (nn) 0.029 0.169 0.17 
BA44 (nd) 0.006 0.070 0.09 
BA44 (vol) 0.075 0.144 0.60 
BA45 (nn) 0.154 0.127 1.21 
BA45(nd) 0.026 0.064 0.42 
BA45 (vol) 0.245 0.127 1.94 
#L#A#RMean AQset
Total volume 
Human 12 11 NA   
Human NA   −0.082 0.035 −2.34* 
Chimpanzee 30 23 −0.104 0.038 −2.71** 
Chimpanzee 87 12 90 0.021 0.030 0.68 
Bonobo −0.097 0.054 −1.77 
Gorilla −0.068 0.023 −2.91 
Gray matter volume 
Human 28 31 0.041 0.061 0.67 
Human 12 18 −0.021 0.112 −0.001 
Human 17 29 .084 0.074 1.13 
Chimpanzee 12 18 0.052 0.066 0.79 
Chimpanzee 87 12 90 0.021 0.030 0.68 
Cytoarchitectonic 
Human 
BA44 10 NA   
BA45 NA   
Chimpanzee 
BA44 (nn) 0.029 0.169 0.17 
BA44 (nd) 0.006 0.070 0.09 
BA44 (vol) 0.075 0.144 0.60 
BA45 (nn) 0.154 0.127 1.21 
BA45(nd) 0.026 0.064 0.42 
BA45 (vol) 0.245 0.127 1.94 

NA, not applicable or no data reported; se, standard error.

More recently, Xiang et al. [61] compared surface area and cortical thickness asymmetries between humans and chimpanzees. In this study, chimpanzee brains were aligned to a human brain template in the software program Freesurfer. Subsequently, the Desikan–Killany atlas maps, developed for use in humans, were subsequently applied to a sample of 77 chimpanzee brain scans and 91 scans of humans. With respect to the Op, Tr, STG and transverse temporal (TT) gyrus, the magnitude and direction of asymmetry were influenced by the different morphological measures (see Figure 4). For surface area, population-level leftward asymmetries were found for both chimpanzees in the superior temporal and TT gyri while no group level bias was found for either the POP or Tr. These results are largely consistent with the previous finding that have used traditional region-of-interest measures. For cortical thickness, no population-level asymmetries were found for either humans or chimpanzees for the Tr and POP regions. In humans, significant rightward asymmetries were found for the superior temporal but not the TT gyrus. In the chimpanzee, significant rightward asymmetries were found for the transverse but not STG.

Figure 4.

Mean AQ scores (±s.e.) for surface area and cortical thickness in the inferior frontal and posterior temporal gyri regions within the Desikan–Killany atlas maps applied to human and chimpanzee brains.

Figure 4.

Mean AQ scores (±s.e.) for surface area and cortical thickness in the inferior frontal and posterior temporal gyri regions within the Desikan–Killany atlas maps applied to human and chimpanzee brains.

Close modal

Surface area of frontal and temporal lobe sulci

As noted above, early studies attempting to quantify brain asymmetries focused on measuring the length of select sulci in different primate species from either endocasts or directly from postmortem brains. In contrast with sulci length, recent studies incorporating MRI scans have quantified sulci surface area and the average depths by extracting cortical folds from the 3D scans using software programs such as Brainvisa (BV). To date, these approaches have been used to measure asymmetries in sulci within the temporal lobe including the sylvian fissure (SF) and superior temporal sulus (STS). Additional measures of asymmetry have been reported for frontal lobe sulci with the ventrolateral and inferior frontal gyri (see Figure 5).

Figure 5.

Lateral views of a macaque (rhesus monkey), baboon and chimpanzee brain with the frontal and temporal lobe sulci labeled for each species. REC = rectus, ARC = arcuate, IFS = inferior frontal sulcus, PCI = precentral inferior, CS = central sulcus, SF = sylvian fissure, and STS = superior temporal sulcus.

Figure 5.

Lateral views of a macaque (rhesus monkey), baboon and chimpanzee brain with the frontal and temporal lobe sulci labeled for each species. REC = rectus, ARC = arcuate, IFS = inferior frontal sulcus, PCI = precentral inferior, CS = central sulcus, SF = sylvian fissure, and STS = superior temporal sulcus.

Close modal

With respect to the frontal lobe, Bogart et al. [62] measured the surface area and depth of the rectus (REC) and arcuate (ARC) in a sample of rhesus (n = 21) and bonnet (n = 28) macaque monkeys. Neither species showed a population bias for either folds (see Table 3). Becker et al. [63] measured the surface area and average depth of the ARC in 50 baboons and found no overall population-level bias (but see Discussion). The sulci that define the IFG include the fronto-orbital (FO), precentral inferior (PCI) and inferior frontal sulci (IFS). Using the software program BV, the surface areas and mean depth of the FO, PCI and IFS were quantified in the left and right hemispheres in 294 chimpanzees. For both surface area and depth, significant leftward asymmetries were found for the IFS and FO sulci but not PCI (see Table 3). In addition, the dorsal portion of the FO sulcus was reported to be significantly more often bifurcated in the left compared with the right hemisphere, a result consistent with a previous study describing the sulci patterns of the IFG in postmortem chimpanzee brains [64].

Table 3.
Asymmetries in surface area of sulci within the inferior frontal gyrus and ventral lateral cortex.
#L#A#RMean AQset
Sulci surface area 
Chimpanzee 
Fronto-orbital (FO) 170 52 96 −0.041 0.011 −3.58** 
Precentral inferior (PCI) 136 40 142 −0.019 0.010 −1.94 
Inferior frontal (IFS) 177 27 114 −0.142 0.031 −4.55** 
Sylvian fissure (SF) 152 69 70 −0.032 0.006 −5.33** 
Superior temporal sulcus (STS) 110 51 138 0.027 0.009 3.00** 
Baboon 
Arcuate (ARC) 22 20 0.004 0.019 0.20 
Rhesus monkey 
Arcuate (ARC) 12 0.080 0.045 1.76 
Rectus (REC) 10 −0.012 0.123 −0.09 
Sylvian fissure (SF) 12 0.060 0.043 −1.40 
Superior temporal sulcus (STS) 13 0.016 0.023 0.68 
Bonnet Monkeys 
Arcuate (ARC) 12 15 0.061 0.086 0.71 
Rectus (REC) 12 14 0.122 0.106 1.15 
Sylvian fissure (SF) 10 16 0.052 0.075 0.69 
Superior temporal sulcus (STS) 20 0.092 0.033 2.28* 
#L#A#RMean AQset
Sulci surface area 
Chimpanzee 
Fronto-orbital (FO) 170 52 96 −0.041 0.011 −3.58** 
Precentral inferior (PCI) 136 40 142 −0.019 0.010 −1.94 
Inferior frontal (IFS) 177 27 114 −0.142 0.031 −4.55** 
Sylvian fissure (SF) 152 69 70 −0.032 0.006 −5.33** 
Superior temporal sulcus (STS) 110 51 138 0.027 0.009 3.00** 
Baboon 
Arcuate (ARC) 22 20 0.004 0.019 0.20 
Rhesus monkey 
Arcuate (ARC) 12 0.080 0.045 1.76 
Rectus (REC) 10 −0.012 0.123 −0.09 
Sylvian fissure (SF) 12 0.060 0.043 −1.40 
Superior temporal sulcus (STS) 13 0.016 0.023 0.68 
Bonnet Monkeys 
Arcuate (ARC) 12 15 0.061 0.086 0.71 
Rectus (REC) 12 14 0.122 0.106 1.15 
Sylvian fissure (SF) 10 16 0.052 0.075 0.69 
Superior temporal sulcus (STS) 20 0.092 0.033 2.28* 

Regarding the temporal lobe sulci, chimpanzees showed a leftward asymmetry for the SF while neither the rhesus nor bonnet monkeys showed a population-level bias (see Table 3). For the STS, chimpanzees showed an overall rightward population-level bias as did the bonnet but not the rhesus monkeys. One interesting observation of the rightward STS asymmetry from the chimpanzee sample was the evidence that the rightward was particularly prominent in the area of the STS referred as the STAP (superior temporal asymmetric pit) [65]. In humans, significant and robust rightward asymmetries have been reported and it was hypothesized to be uniquely human (LeRoy et al. [66]). Contrary to this claim, chimpanzees showed a significant rightward asymmetry in the STAP region, though the magnitude of the bias was much smaller than data reported for human subjects (see Figure 6).

Figure 6.

Mean AQ scores (±s.e.) for the STAP region in chimpanzees. The AQ values represent difference in the depth of the STS in 20 equally spaced anterior to posterior regions.

Figure 6.

Mean AQ scores (±s.e.) for the STAP region in chimpanzees. The AQ values represent difference in the depth of the STS in 20 equally spaced anterior to posterior regions.

Close modal

For the PT, IFG and sulci measures in the chimpanzees, the data were derived from two independent samples of captive apes. Specifically, one set of MRI data were obtained from chimpanzees housed at the National Center for Chimpanzee Care (NCCC) whereas the second cohort was collected in apes housed at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center (YNPRC). These two captive populations of apes were derived from distinct founder animals and there was no interbreeding that took place between individuals between the two populations. Thus, this arrangement provides an opportunity to compare the patterns of asymmetry between these two cohorts as a means of assessing their repeatability. As can be seen in Figure 7, patterns of asymmetry for the PT, IFG and the frontal and temporal lobe sulci were evident in both samples of chimpanzees suggesting the results are repeatable.

Figure 7.

Mean AQ scores (±s.e.) for different measures of brain asymmetry in two independent samples of chimpanzees. NCCC = National Center for Chimpanzee Care (NCCC), YNPRC = Yerkes National Primate Research Center. Left panel represents PT surface area, gray matter volume and IFG gray matter volume. Right panel represents frontal and temporal lobe sulci surface area measures.

Figure 7.

Mean AQ scores (±s.e.) for different measures of brain asymmetry in two independent samples of chimpanzees. NCCC = National Center for Chimpanzee Care (NCCC), YNPRC = Yerkes National Primate Research Center. Left panel represents PT surface area, gray matter volume and IFG gray matter volume. Right panel represents frontal and temporal lobe sulci surface area measures.

Close modal

At the most basic level, and in contrast with many historical views, population-level asymmetries are evident in chimpanzees and, to a lesser extent, other nonhuman primate species. The most robust and consistent population-level asymmetry in chimpanzees and seemingly in baboons is the leftward bias evident in the PT. This finding has been reported in both species using multiple methods of analyses and at different levels of analysis including surface area and gray matter volume. Chimpanzees also show a leftward asymmetry in the volume and neuron density of the BA22 but there are no available data from baboons. It appears the rhesus monkeys may show a population-level leftward asymmetry at the cellular level for BA22 but this sample was relatively small and therefore the results should be interpreted cautiously, particularly in light of the lack of population-level bias in morphological measures from larger samples of subjects. Within the IFG, the evidence of population-level asymmetries seems less clear and appears to be high dependent on the type of measurement (i.e. sulci surface areas versus gray matter volume) and method of assessment. Performing comparative studies of brain asymmetry have several challenges and addressing them in future research would greatly benefit the field and theory. Three considerations for future research are discussed below.

The confounding impact of handedness on studies of comparative brain asymmetry

As noted above, humans are predominantly and strongly right-handed whereas the evidence for population-level handedness in nonhuman primates is far less prevalent and, when evident, not nearly as strongly expressed [4,67,68]. For example, population-level right handedness has been reported in chimpanzees for multiple measures of hand use but the proportion of right-handed individuals approximates 65–70% compared with the 85–90% often reported in human subjects [18,19,69–71]. Because neuroanatomical asymmetries can be associated with handedness [72–77], the comparison of brain asymmetries between primate species is more complicated. Notably, in most studies of human subjects, the majority if not all of the subjects are right-handed whereas the handedness of subjects in many studies with nonhuman primates are either unknown or not reported. In instances where handedness was known in studies with nonhuman primates, significant brain-behavior associations have been reported in some but not all cases [78–84]. For instance, with respect to the ARC sulcus, Becker et al. [63] found that asymmetries in the depth of this fold were significantly more leftward in baboons that communicated gesturally more often with their right compared with the left hand. Similarly, Taglialatela et al. [85] found that chimpanzees that preferred to gesture with their right hand showed greater leftward asymmetries in the IFG compared with individuals that preferred their left hand or had no hand preference bias. In contrast, in both chimpanzees and baboons, hand preferences for bimanual actions were associated with asymmetries in the motor hand area of the precentral gyrus. These findings illustrate two important points. First, without considering or controlling for the handedness of subjects when comparing brain asymmetries between species, it is possible that the results might be biased due to an over representation of right- (or left-handed) individuals within one or more species. Second, at least in nonhuman primates, the type of measure of handedness appears to be associated with asymmetries in different cortical regions depending on their motor and communicative functions. This type of distinction in different dimensions of handedness is not typically considered when testing for association with brain asymmetries in humans.

Brain size, effect size and sample size

Among primates, the human brain is ∼28% of its total volume at birth, which is much smaller compared with other primate species [86]. Additionally, compared with other primates, humans have a particularly long period of infant and juvenile periods of development. The relatively immature brain coupled with the longer period of infancy makes the human brain more susceptible to the influence of early social and environmental input on cortical development, including asymmetry [87–89]. Specifically, human and nonhuman primate newborns are not born into a symmetrical world. From birth (and arguably prior to birth), infants experience asymmetrical sensory and motor experiences, such as head orientation and nipple preferences by the infant and cradling biases by the primary caregiver which arguably can impact the development of both directional and absolute behavioral and brain asymmetries [90–100]. This is important for two reasons. First, depending on their consistency across individuals, the long-term impact of early asymmetrical input on the brain may result in more consistent and robust asymmetries in humans compared with other primate species. Thus, all other factors being equal, estimated effect sizes for the detection of population-level asymmetries will be greater in humans compared with other primate species. This, in turn, would be mean fewer subjects would be needed to detect population-level biases in humans compared with other primates. Samples sizes are already relatively small in many studies on asymmetries in nonhuman primates; therefore, it is possible that population-level asymmetries may be evident in nonhuman primate species for some measures but these studies are underpowered given the estimated smaller effect size.

Whole brain approaches: templates (biased and unbiased)

Lastly, though not reviewed here, it is clear that voxel and surface-based whole brain approaches to the study of brain asymmetry in nonhuman primates are becoming increasingly evident in the literature [61,101–104]. There are many advantages to these approaches in the context of comparative studies of asymmetry in human and nonhuman primates. Notably, it allows for comparison in broadly defined brain regions between species without reliance on common sulci or related landmarks. That stated, there are some potential pitfalls that merit discussion. Notably, in whole brain approaches, the individual MRI scans through many registration steps are warped into a common stereotaxic space which is normally an ‘average’ or template brain for the species. The challenge is that inherent asymmetries in the template brain can exists which can potentially introduce biases in image registration and the measurement of individual asymmetries. Save the recent study by Xiang et al. [61], previous studies using voxel- or surfaced-based whole brain measures of asymmetries in nonhuman primates have not adopted preprocessing or analytic methods that assure that the template brains are indeed symmetrical.

Summary

In sum, in many ways, the study of brain asymmetries in nonhuman primates is still in its infancy. Modern imaging technologies have allowed for scientists to increase the scope and complexity in the study of brain asymmetries at multiple levels of analysis. Compared with studies focused on postmortem tissue, modern imaging also allows for increased samples sizes which has historically and continues to be a significant limitation, particularly in light of the fact that the effect sizes may be smaller in the smaller brained nonhuman primates. At the same time, there remains a need for additional studies examining brain asymmetries at the cellular level of analysis, including in human brains. Lastly, like other aspects of cortical function in relation to complex behavior and cognition, lateralization in function likely operates through a series of connected nodes rather than discrete, isolated brain regions [105–110]. Thus, it is possible (and likely) that specific functions might be governed by a series of lateralized nodes that may be connected within and between hemispheres. Thus, studies may benefit by considering a comparative analysis of functional or anatomical connectivity rather than the whole brain or region-of-interest analyses.

  • Chimpanzees are an excellent model species for comparison to humans in the evolution of brain asymmetries.

  • Individual and population-level leftward asymmetries are evident in the PT of chimpanzees at multiple levels of analysis. Similar leftward asymmetries appear evident in baboons but it less consistent in other Old World monkey species.

  • Despite initial reports of leftward volumetric asymmetries in the homolog of Broca's area, evidence of population-level asymmetries is less clear cut in chimpanzees and humans.

The author declares that there are no competing interests associated with this manuscript.

This work was supported, in part, by NIH grants AG-067419, NS-073134, NS-42867 and NS-092988.

Open access for this article was enabled by the participation of University of Texas M.D. Anderson Cancer Centre in an all-inclusive Read & Publish agreement with Portland Press and the Biochemical Society.

AQ

asymmetry quotient

ARC

arcuate

BA22

Brodmann's area 22

BV

Brainvisa

FO

fronto-orbital

IFG

inferior frontal gyrus

Ifs

inferior frontal sulcus

PCI

precentral inferior

PT

planum temporale

REC

rectus

SF

sylvian fissure

STAP

superior temporal asymmetric pit

STG

superior temporal gyrus

STS

superior temporal sulus

1
Corballis
,
M.C.
(
1992
)
The Lopsided Brain: Evolution of the Generative Mind
,
Oxford University Press
,
New York, NY
2
McManus
,
C.
(
2002
)
Right Hand, Left Hand: the Origins of Asymmetry in Brains, Bodies, Atoms, and Cultures
,
Weidenfeld & Nicolson
,
London
3
Rogers
,
L.J.
,
Vallortigara
,
G.
and
Andrew
,
R.J.
(
2013
)
Divided Brains: the Biology and Behaviour of Brain Asymmetries
,
Cambridge University Press
,
New York
4
Ocklenburg
,
S.
and
Gunturkun
,
O.
(
2018
)
The Lateralized Brain: the Neuroscience and Evolution of Hemispheric Asymmetries
,
Academic Press
,
London
5
Saenger
,
V.M.
,
Barrios
,
F.A.
,
Martinez-Gudino
,
M.L.
and
Alcauter
,
S.
(
2012
)
Hemispheric asymmetries of functional connectivity and grey matter volume in the default mode network
.
Neuropsychologia
50
,
1308
1315
6
Toga
,
A.W.
and
Thompson
,
P.M.
(
2003
)
Mapping brain asymmetry
.
Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
4
,
37
48
7
Luders
,
E.
,
Gaser
,
C.
,
Jancke
,
L.
and
Schlaug
,
G.
(
2004
)
A voxel-based approach to gray matter asymmetries
.
Neuroimage
22
,
656
664
8
Luders
,
E.
,
Narr
,
K.L.
,
Thompson
,
P.M.
,
Rex
,
D.E.
,
Jancke
,
L.
and
Toga
,
A.W.
(
2006
)
Hemispheric asymmetries in cortical thickness
.
Cereb Cortex
16
,
1232
1238
9
Buchel
,
C.
,
Raedler
,
T.
,
Sommer
,
M.
,
Sach
,
M.
,
Weiller
,
C.
and
Koch
,
M.A.
(
2004
)
White matter asymmetry in the human brain: a diffusion tensor MRI study
.
Cereb Cortex
14
,
945
951
10
LeMay
,
M.
(
1985
) Asymmetries of the brains and skulls of nonhuman primates. In
Cerebral Lateralization in Nonhuman Species
(
Glick
,
S.D.
, ed.), pp.
223
245
,
Academic Press
,
New York
11
Warren
,
J.M.
(
1980
)
Handedness and laterality in humans and other animals
.
Physiol. Psychol.
8
,
351
359
12
Ettlinger
,
G.F.
(
1988
)
Hand preference, ability and hemispheric specialization. How far are these factors related in the monkey?
Cortex
24
,
389
398
13
Finch
,
G.
(
1941
)
Chimpanzee handedness
.
Science
94
,
117
118
14
Macneilage
,
P.F.
,
Studdertkennedy
,
M.G.
and
Lindblom
,
B.
(
1987
)
Primate handedness reconsidered
.
Behav. Brain Sci.
10
,
247
263
15
McGrew
,
W.C.
and
Marchant
,
L.F.
(
1997
)
On the other hand: current issues in and meta-analysis of the behavioral laterality of hand function in non-human primates
.
Am. J. Phys. Anthropol.
104
,
201
232
16
Hopkins
,
W.D.
and
Cantalupo
,
C.
(
2005
)
Individual and setting differences in the hand preferences of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): a critical analysis and some alternative explanations
.
Laterality
10
,
65
80
17
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Pika
,
S.
,
Liebal
,
K.
,
Bania
,
A.
,
Meguerditchian
,
A.
,
Gardner
,
M.
et al, (
2012
) Handedness for manual gestures in great apes: a meta-analysis. In
Current Developments in non-Human Primate Gesture Research
(
Pika
,
S.
and
Liebal
,
K.
, eds), pp.
93
111
John Benjamins Publishing
,
Amsterdam
18
Hopkins
,
W.D.
(
2013
)
Comparing human and nonhuman primate handedness: challenges and a modest proposal for concensus
.
Dev. Psychobiol.
55
,
621
636
19
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Gardner
,
M.
,
Mingle
,
M.
,
Reamer
,
L.
and
Schapiro
,
S.J.
(
2013
)
Within- and between-task consistency in hand use as a means of characterizing hand preferences in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
.
J. Comp. Psychol.
127
,
380
391
20
Hopkins
,
W.D.
(
2006
)
Comparative and familial analysis of handedness in great apes
.
Psychol. Bull.
132
,
538
559
21
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Misiura
,
M.
,
Pope
,
S.M.
and
Latash
,
E.M.
(
2015
)
Behavioral and brain asymmetries in primates: a preliminary evaluation of two evolutionary hypotheses
.
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.
1359
,
65
83
22
Yeni-Komshian
,
G.H.
and
Benson
,
D.A.
(
1976
)
Anatomical study of cerebral asymmetry in the temporal lobe of humans, chimpanzees, and rhesus monkeys
.
Science
192
,
387
389
23
Heilbronner
,
P.L.
and
Holloway
,
R.L.
(
1988
)
Anatomical brain asymmetries in New World and Old World monkeys. Stages of temporal lobe development in primate evolution
.
Am. J. Phys. Anthropol.
76
,
39
48
24
Heilbronner
,
P.L.
and
Holloway
,
R.L.
(
1989
)
Anatomical brain asymmetry in monkeys: frontal, temporoparietal, and limbic cortex in Macaca
.
Am. J. Phys. Anthropol.
80
,
203
211
25
Suwada
,
K.
(
2020
)
Cerebral sulcal asymmetry in macaque monkeys
.
Symmetry
12
,
1
9
26
Imai
,
N.
,
Sawada
,
K.
,
Fukunishi
,
K.
,
Sakata-Haga
,
H.
and
Fukui
,
Y.
(
2011
)
Sexual dimorphism of sulcal length asymmetry in the cerebrum of adult cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis)
.
Congenit. Anom. (Kyoto)
51
,
161
166
27
Falk
,
D.
,
Cheverud
,
J.
,
Vannier
,
M.W.
and
Conroy
,
G.C.
(
1986
)
Advanced computer graphics technology reveals cortical asymmetry in endocasts of rhesus monkeys
.
Folia Primatol. (Basel)
46
,
98
103
28
Falk
,
D.
,
Hildebolt
,
C.
,
Cheverud
,
J.
,
Vannier
,
M.
,
Helmkamp
,
R.C.
and
Konigsberg
,
L.
(
1990
)
Cortical asymmetries in the frontal lobe of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)
.
Brain Res.
512
,
40
45
29
Cain
,
D.P.
and
Wada
,
J.A.
(
1979
)
An anatomical asymmetry in the baboon brain
.
Brain Behav. Evol.
16
,
222
226
30
Liu
,
S.T.
and
Phillips
,
K.A.
(
2009
)
Sylvian fissure asymmetry in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)
.
Laterality
14
,
217
227
31
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Pilcher
,
D.L.
and
MacGregor
,
L.
(
2000
)
Sylvian fissure length asymmetries in primates revisited: a comparative MRI study
.
Brain Behav. Evol.
56
,
293
299
32
Fears
,
S.C.
,
Scheibel
,
K.
,
Abaryan
,
Z.
,
Lee
,
C.
,
Service
,
S.K.
,
Jorgensen
,
M.J.
et al (
2011
)
Anatomic brain asymmetry in vervet monkeys
.
PLoS One
6
,
e28243
33
Holloway
,
R.L.
and
Delacostelareymondie
,
M.C.
(
1982
)
Brain endocast asymmetry in pongids and hominids - some preliminary findings on the paleontology of cerebral-dominance
.
Am. J. Phys. Anthropol.
58
,
101
110
34
Balzeau
,
A.
,
Gilissen
,
E.
,
Holloway
,
R.L.
,
Prima
,
S.
and
Grimaud-Herve
,
D.
(
2014
)
Variations in size, shape and asymmetries of the third frontal convolution in hominids: paleoneurological implications for hominin evolution and the origin of language
.
J. Hum. Evol.
76
,
116
128
35
Neubauer
,
S.
,
Gunz
,
P.
and
Hublin
,
J.J.
(
2010
)
Endocranial shape changes during growth in chimpanzees and humans: a morphometric analysis of unique and shared aspects
.
J. Hum. Evol.
59
,
555
566
36
Brodmann
,
K.
(
1909
)
Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grobhirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues
,
Barth
,
Leipzig
37
Wernicke
,
C.
(
1874
)
Der aphasische Symptomenkomplex
,
Cohn, Weigert
,
Breslau
38
Gannon
,
P.J.
,
Holloway
,
R.L.
,
Broadfield
,
D.C.
and
Braun
,
A.R.
(
1998
)
Asymmetry of chimpanzee planum temporale: humanlike pattern of Wernicke's brain language area homolog
.
Science
279
,
220
222
39
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Marino
,
L.
,
Rilling
,
J.K.
and
MacGregor
,
L.A.
(
1998
)
Planum temporale asymmetries in great apes as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)
.
Neuroreport
9
,
2913
2918
40
Spocter
,
M.A.
,
Sherwood
,
C.C.
,
Schapiro
,
S.J.
and
Hopkins
,
W.D.
(
2020
)
Reproducibility of leftward planum temporale asymmetries in two genetically isolated populations of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
.
Proc. Biol. Sci.
287
,
20201320
41
Hopkins
,
W.D.
and
Nir
,
T.
(
2010
)
Planum temporale surface area and grey matter asymmetries in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the effect of handedness and comparison within findings in humans
.
Behav. Brain Res.
208
,
436
443
42
Gilissen
,
E.
, (
2001
) Structural symmetries and asymmetries in human and chimpanzee brains. In
Evolutionary Anatomy of the Primate Cerebral Cortex
(
Falk
,
D.
and
Gibson
,
K.R.
, eds), pp.
187
215
,
Cambridge University
,
Cambridge
43
Marie
,
D.
,
Roth
,
M.
,
Lacoste
,
R.
,
Nazarian
,
B.
,
Bertello
,
A.
,
Anton
,
J.L.
et al (
2018
)
Left brain asymmetry of the planum temporale in a nonhominid primate: redefining the origin of brain specialization for language
.
Cereb Cortex
28
,
1808
1815
44
Becker
,
Y.
,
Sein
,
J.
,
Velly
,
L.
,
Giacomino
,
L.
,
Renaud
,
L.
,
Lacoste
,
R.
et al (
2021
)
Early left-planum temporale asymmetry in newborn monkeys (Papio anubis): a longitudinal structural MRI study at two stages of development
.
Neuroimage
227
,
117575
45
Becker
,
Y.
,
Phelipon
,
R.
,
Sein
,
J.
,
Velly
,
L.
,
Renaud
,
L.
and
Meguerditchian
,
A.
(
2022
)
Planum temporale grey matter volume asymmetries in newborn monkeys (Papio anubis)
.
Brain Struct. Funct.
227
,
463
468
46
Lyn
,
H.
,
Pierre
,
P.
,
Bennett
,
A.J.
,
Fears
,
S.
,
Woods
,
R.
and
Hopkins
,
W.D.
(
2011
)
Planum temporale grey matter asymmetries in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), vervet (Chlorocebus aethiops sabaeus), rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and bonnet (Macaca radiata) monkeys
.
Neuropsychologia
49
,
2004
2012
47
Galaburda
,
A.M.
,
Sanides
,
F.
and
Geschwind
,
N.
(
1978
)
Human brain. Cytoarchitectonic left-right asymmetries in the temporal speech region
.
Arch. Neurol.
35
,
812
817
48
Spocter
,
M.A.
,
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Garrison
,
A.R.
,
Stimpson
,
C.D.
,
Erwin
,
J.M.
,
Hof
,
P.R.
et al (
2010
)
Wernicke's area homolog in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): probabilstic mapping, asymmetry and comparison with humans
.
Proc. R. Soc. B. Biol. Sci.
277
,
2165
2174
49
Spocter
,
M.A.
,
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Barks
,
S.K.
,
Bianchi
,
S.
,
Hehmeyer
,
A.E.
,
Anderson
,
S.M.
et al (
2012
)
Neuropil distribution in the cerebral cortex differs between humans and chimpanzees
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
520
,
2917
2929
50
Gannon
,
P.J.
,
Kheck
,
N.
and
Hof
,
P.R.
(
2008
)
Leftward interhemispheric asymmetry of macaque monkey temporal lobe language area homolog is evident at the cytoarchitectural, but not gross anatomic level
.
Brain Res.
1199
,
62
73
51
Keller
,
S.S.
,
Crow
,
T.
,
Foundas
,
A.
,
Amunts
,
K.
and
Roberts
,
N.
(
2009
)
Broca's area: nomenclature, anatomy, typology and asymmetry
.
Brain Lang.
109
,
29
48
52
Keller
,
S.S.
,
Highley
,
J.R.
,
Garcia-Finana
,
M.
,
Sluming
,
V.
,
Rezaie
,
R.
and
Roberts
,
N.
(
2007
)
Sulcal variability, stereological measurement and asymmetry of Broca's area on MR images
.
J. Anat.
211
,
534
555
53
Petrides
,
M.
(
2019
)
Atlas of the Morphology of the Human Cerebral Cortex on the Average MNI Brain
,
Academic Press
,
New York
54
Palomero-Gallagher
,
N.
and
Zilles
,
K.
(
2019
)
Differences in cytoarchitecture of Broca's region between human, ape and macaque brains
.
Cortex
118
,
132
153
55
Petrides
,
M.
,
Cadoret
,
G.
and
Mackey
,
S.
(
2005
)
Orofacial somatomotor responses in the macaque monkey homologue of Broca's area
.
Nature
435
,
1235
1238
56
Knaus
,
T.A.
,
Bollich
,
A.M.
,
Corey
,
D.M.
,
Lemen
,
L.C.
and
Foundas
,
A.L.
(
2006
)
Variability in perisylvian brain anatomy in healthy adults
.
Brain Lang.
97
,
219
232
57
Tomaiuolo
,
F.
,
MacDonald
,
J.D.
,
Caramanos
,
Z.
,
Posner
,
G.
,
Chiavaras
,
M.
,
Evans
,
A.C.
et al (
1999
)
Morphology, morphometry and probability mapping of the pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus: an in vivo MRI analysis
.
Eur. J. Neurosci.
11
,
3033
3046
58
Keller
,
S.S.
,
Roberts
,
N.
and
Hopkins
,
W.
(
2009
)
A comparative magnetic resonance imaging study of the anatomy, variability, and asymmetry of Broca's area in the human and chimpanzee brain
.
J. Neurosci.
29
,
14607
14616
59
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Meguerditchian
,
A.
,
Coulon
,
O.
,
Misiura
,
M.
,
Pope
,
S.
,
Mareno
,
M.C.
et al (
2017
)
Motor skill for tool-use is associated with asymmetries in Broca's area and the motor hand area of the precentral gyrus in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
.
Behav. Brain Res.
318
,
71
81
60
Cantalupo
,
C.
and
Hopkins
,
W.D.
(
2001
)
Asymmetric Broca's area in great apes
.
Nature
414
,
505
61
Xiang
,
L.
,
Crow
,
T.J.
,
Hopkins
,
W.D.
and
Roberts
,
N.
(
2020
)
Comparison of surface area and cortical thickness asymmetry in the human and chimpanzee brain
.
Cereb Cortex
,
1
15
62
Bogart
,
S.L.
,
Mangin
,
J.F.
,
Schapiro
,
S.J.
,
Reamer
,
L.
,
Bennett
,
A.J.
,
Pierre
,
P.J.
et al (
2012
)
Cortical sulci asymmetries in chimpanzees and macaques: a new look at an old idea
.
Neuroimage
61
,
533
541
63
Becker
,
Y.
,
Claidiere
,
N.
,
Margiotoudi
,
K.
,
Marie
,
D.
,
Roth
,
M.
,
Nazarian
,
B.
et al (
2022
)
Broca's cerebral asymmetry reflects gestural communication's lateralisation in monkeys (Papio anubis)
.
eLife
11
,
e70521
64
Keller
,
S.S.
,
Deppe
,
M.
,
Herbin
,
M.
and
Gilissen
,
E.
(
2012
)
Variabilty and asymmetry of the sulcal contours defining Broca's area homologue in the chimpanzee brain
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
520
,
1165
1180
65
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Coulon
,
O.
,
Meguerditchian
,
A.
,
Staes
,
N.
,
Sherwood
,
C.C.
,
Schapiro
,
S.J.
et al (
2022
)
Genetic determinants of individual variation in the superior temporal sulcus of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
.
Cerebral Cortex
, in press
66
LeRoy
,
F.
,
Cai
,
Q.
,
Bogart
,
S.L.
,
Dubois
,
J.
,
Coulon
,
O.
,
Monzalvo
,
K
et al (
2015
)
New human-specific brain landmark: The depth asymmetry of superior temporal sulcus
.
PNAS
112
,
1208
1213
.
67
Porac
,
C.
and
Coren
,
S.
(
1981
)
Lateral Preferences and Human Behavior
,
Springer
,
New York
68
Annett
,
M.
(
2002
)
Handedness and Brain Asymmetry: the Right Shift Theory
,
Psychology Press
,
Hove
69
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Russell
,
J.L.
,
Cantalupo
,
C.
,
Freeman
,
H.
and
Schapiro
,
S.J.
(
2005
)
Factors influencing the prevalence and handedness for throwing in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
.
J. Comp. Psychol.
119
,
363
370
70
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Russell
,
J.L.
and
Schaeffer
,
J.A.
(
2012
)
The neural and cognitive correlates of aimed throwing in chimpanzees: a magnetic resonance image and behavioural study on a unique form of social tool use
.
Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.
367
,
37
47
71
Papademetriou
,
E.
,
Sheu
,
C.F.
and
Michel
,
G.F.
(
2005
)
A meta-analysis of primate hand preferences for reaching and other hand-use measures
.
J. Comp. Psychol.
119
,
33
48
72
Knecht
,
S.
,
Drager
,
B.
,
Deppe
,
M.
,
Bobe
,
L.
,
Lohmann
,
H.
,
Floel
,
A.
et al (
2000
)
Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans
.
Brain
123
,
2512
2518
73
Foundas
,
A.L.
,
Hong
,
K.
,
Leonard
,
C.M.
and
Heilman
,
K.M.
(
1998
)
Hand preference and magnetic resonance imaging asymmetries of the central sulcus
.
Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol.
11
,
65
71
74
Foundas
,
A.L.
,
Leonard
,
C.M.
and
Hanna-Pladdy
,
B.
(
2002
)
Variability in the anatomy of the planum temporale and posterior ascending ramus: do right- and left handers differ?
Brain Lang.
83
,
403
424
75
Foundas
,
A.L.
,
Leonard
,
C.M.
and
Heilman
,
K.M.
(
1995
)
Morphological cerebral asymmetries and handedness: the pars triangularis and planum temporale
.
Arch. Neurol.
52
,
501
508
76
Sha
,
Z.
,
Pepe
,
A.
,
Schijven
,
D.
,
Carrion-Castillo
,
A.
,
Roe
,
J.M.
,
Westerhausen
,
R.
et al (
2021
)
Handedness and its genetic influences are associated with structural asymmetries of the cerebral cortex in 31,864 individuals
.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
118
77
Zetzsche
,
T.
,
Meisenzahl
,
E.M.
,
Preuss
,
U.W.
,
Holder
,
J.J.
,
Kathmann
,
N.
,
Leinsinger
,
G.
et al (
2001
)
In-vivo analysis of human planum temporale (PT): does the definition of PT borders influence the results with regard to cerebral asymmetry and correlation with handedness?
Psychiatry Res.
107
,
99
115
78
Phillips
,
K.A.
and
Sherwood
,
C.C.
(
2005
)
Primary motor cortex asymmetry is correlated with handedness in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)
.
Behav. Neurosci.
119
,
1701
1704
79
Sherwood
,
C.C.
,
Wahl
,
E.
,
Erwin
,
J.M.
,
Hof
,
P.R.
and
Hopkins
,
W.D.
(
2007
)
Histological asymmetries of primary motor cortex predicts handedness in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
503
,
525
537
80
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Coulon
,
O.
and
Mangin
,
J.F.
(
2010
)
Observer-independent characterization of sulcal landmarks and depth asymmetry in the central sulcus of the chimpanzee brain
.
Neuroscience
171
,
544
551
81
Hopkins
,
W.D.
and
Cantalupo
,
C.
(
2004
)
Handedness in chimpanzees is associated with asymmetries in the primary motor but not with homologous language areas
.
Behav. Neurosci.
118
,
1176
1183
82
Meguerditchian
,
A.
,
Gardner
,
M.J.
,
Schapiro
,
S.J.
and
Hopkins
,
W.D.
(
2012
)
The sound of one-hand clapping: handedness and perisylvian neural correlates of a communicative gesture in chimpanzees
.
Proc. Biol. Sci.
279
,
1959
1966
83
Hamilton
,
C.R.
and
Vermeire
,
B.A.
(
1988
)
Complementary hemispheric specialization in monkeys
.
Science
242
,
1691
1694
84
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Taglialatela
,
J.P.
,
Dunham
,
L.
and
Pierre
,
P.
(
2007
)
Behavioral and neuroanatomical correlates of white matter asymmetries in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
.
Eur. J. Neurosci.
25
,
2565
2570
85
Taglialatela
,
J.P.
,
Cantalupo
,
C.
and
Hopkins
,
W.D.
(
2006
)
Gesture handedness predicts asymmetry in the chimpanzee inferior frontal gyrus
.
Neuroreport
17
,
923
927
86
Leigh
,
S.R.
(
2004
)
Brain growth, life history, and cognition in primate and human evolution
.
Am. J. Primatol.
62
,
139
164
87
Gomez-Robles
,
A.
,
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Schapiro
,
S.J.
and
Sherwood
,
C.C.
(
2015
)
Relaxed genetic control of cortical organization in human brains compared with chimpanzees
.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
112
,
14799
14804
88
Gomez-Robles
,
A.
,
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Schapiro
,
S.J.
and
Sherwood
,
C.C.
(
2016
)
The heritability of chimpanzee and human brain asymmetry
.
Proc. Biol. Sci.
283
,
20161319
89
Gomez-Robles
,
A.
,
Hopkins
,
W.D.
and
Sherwood
,
C.C.
(
2014
)
Modular structure facilitates mosaic evolution of the brain in chimpanzees and humans
.
Nat. Commun.
5
,
4469
90
Hepper
,
P.G.
,
Wells
,
D.L.
and
Lynch
,
C.
(
2005
)
Prenatal thumb sucking is related to postnatal handedness
.
Neuropsychologia
43
,
313
315
91
Previc
,
F.H.
(
1991
)
A general theory concerning the prenatal origins of cerebral lateralization in humans
.
Psychol. Rev.
98
,
299
334
92
Michel
,
G.F.
,
Campbell
,
J.M.
,
Marcinowski
,
E.C.
,
Nelson
,
E.L.
Babik
,
I.
(
2016
)
Infant hand preference and the development of cognitive abilities
.
Front. Psychol.
7
93
Hopkins
,
W.D.
(
2004
)
Laterality in maternal cradling and infant positional biases: implications for the development and evolution of hand preferences in nonhuman primates
.
Int. J. Primatol.
25
,
1243
1265
94
Regaiolli
,
B.
,
Spiezio
,
C.
and
Hopkins
,
W.D.
(
2018
)
Asymmetries in mother-infant behaviour in Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus)
.
PeerJ
6
,
e4736
95
Damerose
,
E.
and
Hopkins
,
W.D.
(
2002
)
A comparison of scan and focal sampling procedures in the assessment of laterality for maternal cradling and infant nipple preferences in olive baboons (Papio anubis)
.
Anim. Behav.
63
,
511
518
96
Hopkins
,
W.D.
and
De Lathouwers
,
M.
(
2006
)
Left nipple preferences in infant Pan paniscus and P. troglodytes
.
Int. J. Primatol.
27
,
1653
1662
97
Jaffe
,
B.D.
,
Evans
,
T.A.
,
Howell
,
S.
,
Westergaard
,
G.C.
,
Snoy
,
P.J.
,
Higley
,
J.D.
(
2006
)
Left versus right nipple preference in free-ranging infant rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)
.
Dev. Psychobiol.
48
,
266
272
98
Nishida
,
T.
(
1993
)
Left nipple suckling preference in wild chimpanzees
.
Ethol. Sociobiol.
14
,
45
51
99
Zhao
,
D.
,
Gao
,
X.
,
Li
,
B.
and
Watanabe
,
K.
(
2008
)
First wild evidence of neonate nipple preference and maternal cradling laterality in Old World monkeys: a preliminary study from Rhinopithecus roxellana
.
Behav. Process.
77
,
364
368
100
Harris
,
L.J.
(
2010
)
Side biases for holding and carrying infants: reports from the past and possible lessons for today
.
Laterality
15
,
56
135
101
Vickery
,
S.
,
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Sherwood
,
C.C.
,
Schapiro
,
S.J.
,
Latzman
,
R.D.
,
Caspers
,
S.
et al (
2020
)
Chimpanzee brain morphometry utilizing standardized MRI preprocessing and macroanatomical annotations
.
eLife
9
,
e60136
102
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Taglialatela
,
J.P.
,
Meguerditchian
,
A.
,
Nir
,
T.
,
Schenker
,
N.M.
and
Sherwood
,
C.C.
(
2008
)
Gray matter asymmetries in chimpanzees as revealed by voxel-based morphometry
.
Neuroimage
42
,
491
497
103
Hopkins
,
W.D.
,
Taglialatela
,
J.P.
,
Nir
,
T.
,
Schenker
,
N.M.
and
Sherwood
,
C.C.
(
2010
)
A voxel-based morphometry analysis of white matter asymmetries in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
.
Brain Behav. Evol.
76
,
93
100
104
Xia
,
J.
,
Wang
,
F.
,
Wu
,
Z.
,
Wang
,
L.
,
Zhang
,
C.
,
Shen
,
D.
et al (
2020
)
Mapping hemispheric asymmetries of the macaque cerebral cortex during early brain development
.
Hum. Brain Mapp.
41
,
95
106
105
Raemaekers
,
M.
,
Schellekens
,
W.
,
Petridou
,
N.
and
Ramsey
,
N.F.
(
2018
)
Knowing left from right: asymmetric functional connectivity during resting state
.
Brain Struct. Funct.
223
,
1909
1922
106
Liu
,
H.S.
,
Stufflebeam
,
S.M.
,
Sepulcre
,
J.
,
Hedden
,
T.
and
Buckner
,
R.L.
(
2009
)
Evidence from intrinsic activity that asymmetry of the human brain is controlled by multiple factors
.
Proc. Nat Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
106
,
20499
20503
107
Joliot
,
M.
,
Tzourio-Mazoyer
,
N.
and
Mazoyer
,
B.
(
2016
)
Intra-hemispheric intrinsic connectivity asymmetry and its relationships with handedness and language lateralization
.
Neuropsychologia
93
,
437
447
108
Yan
,
H.
,
Zuo
,
X.N.
,
Wang
,
D.
,
Wang
,
J.
,
Zhu
,
C.
,
Milham
,
M.P.
, (
2009
)
Hemispheric asymmetry in cognitive division of anterior cingulate cortex: a resting-state functional connectivity study
.
Neuroimage
47
,
1579
1589
109
Mechelli
,
A.
,
Friston
,
K.J.
,
Frackowiak
,
R.S.
and
Price
,
C.J.
(
2005
)
Structural covariance in the human cortex
.
J. Neurosci.
25
,
8303
8310
110
Cheng
,
L.
,
Zhang
,
Y.
,
Li
,
G.
,
Wang
,
J.
,
Sherwood
,
C.
,
Gong
,
G.
et al (
2021
)
Connectional asymmetry of the inferior parietal lobule shapes hemispheric specialization in humans, chimpanzees, and rhesus macaques
.
eLife
10
,
e67600
This is an open access article published by Portland Press Limited on behalf of the Biochemical Society and the Royal Society of Biology and distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License 4.0 (CC BY). Open access for this article was enabled by the participation of University of Texas M.D. Anderson Cancer Centre in an all-inclusive Read & Publish agreement with Portland Press and the Biochemical Society.